Exercise Psychology
KIN 366 is a course designed to cover: (a) psychological theories for understanding and predicting health-oriented exercise behavior; (b) psychological and psychobiological responses to exercise; (c) psychological interventions for increasing exercise participation and adherence.
KIN 366 is a required course for several options in the undergraduate Kinesiology curriculum and is usually highly rated by students.
Topics include:
- Short introduction to the course
- Why does the world need exercise psychology?
- Fundamentals of research
- Exercise prescription trends
- Acute effects on state anxiety and mood
- Mechanisms of acute "feel-better" effects
- Acute affective responses: Dose-response
- Dual-mode theory
- Instructions for Evidence-Based Practice Project
- Rating of Perceived Exertion
- Chronic exercise and mental health
- Exercise and personality
- Exercise and cognitive function
- Health Belief Model
- Theory of Reasoned Action and Theory of Planned Behavior
- Social Cognitive Theory
- Transtheoretical Model
- Correlates of physical activity
- Physical activity interventions
Some video clips from KIN 366: Exercise Psychology |
---|
A study on the perceived effectiveness of antidepressant treatments. How is exercise rated by individuals diagnosed with clinical depression? |
As exercise intensity approaches an individual's maximal capacity, the prefrontal cortex is deactivated. Why does this happen? |
How do you help an individual who is out-of-shape and feels bad during even modest intensities of exercise? |
In the case of interoceptive stimuli, such as those generated during exercise, the amygdala can be reached via multiple "low roads." Why? |
Pleasure and displeasure evolved because they served some function that was important for survival. |
Bodily movement, physical activity has been an integral part of human evolution. |
When you give individuals the autonomy to self-select their exercise intensity, most will pick an appropriate level but some will pick intensities that are either too low to be effective or too high to be safe. |
What are the predictions of the Dual Mode Theory about the relation between different levels of exercise intensity and affective responses (i.e., pleasure vs. displeasure)? |
A common adage in exercise science and the popular press is that "exercise makes people feel better." But is this really true for most people? |
The Profile of Mood States (POMS) is one of the most popular measures in exercise psychology. But few of the researchers who use it are aware of its history. |
Oftentimes, exercise programs are promoted on the basis of short-term adaptations in fitness or health. But the only really effective exercise intervention is the one that can be sustained over the long haul. |
Exercise fads, such as High-Intensity Interval Training, are promoted on the basis of promises that you only need to do "a little bit" of exercise. What are the psychological implications of these promises? |
Most people who start exercise programs drop out. While it may be comforting to exercise professionals to think that this is a failure on the part of the individual who quit, it is in fact the responsibility of the exercise professional to help prevent the dropout. |
In the mid-1990s, the focus of exercise science started to shift, from recommending regimented exercise programs to recommending incorporating more physical activity into people's lifestyles. |
Perceived lack of discretionary or leisure time is reliably reported as the leading cause of physical inactivity. But is this claim really true? |
On August 17, 2009, TIME magazine cover story was entitled "The Myth About Exercise." Let's look at the portrayal of exercise in that article. |